特黄特色三级在线观看免费,看黄色片子免费,色综合久,欧美在线视频看看,高潮胡言乱语对白刺激国产,伊人网成人,中文字幕亚洲一碰就硬老熟妇

學(xué)習(xí)啦 > 學(xué)習(xí)英語 > 英語閱讀 > 英語美文欣賞 > 有關(guān)于狗狗的英語美文

有關(guān)于狗狗的英語美文

時(shí)間: 韋彥867 分享

有關(guān)于狗狗的英語美文

  作為老師,應(yīng)根據(jù)學(xué)生的知識(shí)水平與理解能力,把教育的話題與隱含著教育內(nèi)容的美文相互整合,把教育內(nèi)容融合在相關(guān)美文中,引導(dǎo)學(xué)生在感悟美文之余受到思想的感染、熏陶與啟迪。學(xué)習(xí)啦小編整理了有關(guān)于狗狗的英語美文,歡迎閱讀!

  有關(guān)于狗狗的英語美文篇一

  The origin of dogs

  Have you ever taken a good look at your dog and wondered, where did this creature come from?

  你是否曾仔細(xì)觀察過你的狗狗,迫切想知道汪星人這種生物到底來自哪里?

  Sure, you know your dog came from the local shelter or a pet store, but what about before that?

  當(dāng)然,你清楚它們是來自當(dāng)?shù)乇茈y所或?qū)櫸锷痰?但是在這之前呢?

  If you were able to trace your dog's lineage back to its origins, what would you find?

  如果你能夠跟蹤狗狗的血統(tǒng)回到它的起源,你會(huì)有何發(fā)現(xiàn)?

  Until recently, scientists believed that domestic dogs originated in the Middle East.

  直到最近,科學(xué)家們相信國內(nèi)的狗起源于中東地區(qū)。

  But reports suggest that almost all domestic dogs began in East Asia as the offspring of three lineages.

  但報(bào)告顯示,幾乎所有國內(nèi)的狗都是東亞血統(tǒng)三代之后。

  Virtually all domesticated dogs in the United States descend from dogs brought over by ancient people that crossed the Bering land bridge from Asia to North America.

  幾乎所有美國家養(yǎng)的狗狗都是從亞洲到北美穿過白令海峽大陸橋的古代人們帶過來繁衍生息下來的子孫。

  Although the New World had a healthy wolf population that might have given rise to a New World strain of domesticated dogs, by and large these ancient immigrants stuck with the dogs of Asian origin.

  雖然新世界繁殖良好的狼可能會(huì)為國內(nèi)狗狗帶來新的壓力,但總的來說,這些古代移民無法擺脫自己的亞裔血統(tǒng)。

  How, you may wonder, have scientists come to these conclusions?

  怎么樣,你是不是會(huì)覺得奇怪,科學(xué)家哪來的這些結(jié)論?

  The same way forensic specialists increasingly solve crimes-through DNA evidence.

  這就如同法醫(yī)專家通過DNA證據(jù)破案一樣。

  Since mitochondria are cellular elements passed from mother to pup, mitochondrial DNA readily reveal genetic footprints stretching back into prehistoric times.

  線粒體是通過狗母親傳遞的細(xì)胞元素,線粒體DNA揭示伸展回到史前時(shí)代的遺傳印記。

  Scientists from the Smithsonian Institution in Washington, D.C. collected DNA samples from the remains of ancient Old World dogs, ancient New World dogs, and ancient New World wolves.

  華盛頓特區(qū)史密森學(xué)會(huì)的科學(xué)家們從古老舊世界的狗狗,古代新世界及古代的狼的遺體中收集DNA樣本。

  Test showed decisively that the New World dogs were genetically more similar to Old World dogs than to New World wolves, effectively proving that the dogs we save from local shelters originally hail from the far East rather than the woods of North America.

  試驗(yàn)表明新世界狗狗的基因相比新世界的狼更類似于舊世界,這有力的證明我們從當(dāng)?shù)乇茈y所中救助的狗狗最初來自遠(yuǎn)東而非北美的森林。

  有關(guān)于狗狗的英語美文篇二

  Fly Dogs

  Air travel makes some people very nervous. The crowds, the noise and flying itself can causeunease. But there are classes people can take to help them defeat the fear of flying. And, nowCaty Weaver tells us about similar training for service dogs that suffer the same problem.

  Service dogs almost never leave the side of the people they care for. You will see them workingon buses, trains and other public transport systems. But the busy environment found at anairport can trouble even the best trained working dog.

  People with disabilities depend on their dogs. They want the animals to safely guide themthrough security areas. They also need to the dogs to remain calmly on duty on the airplane,even when the flying is not smooth. This takes special training. The Air Hollywood K9 FlightSchool is one place where such help can be found.

  The school has a piece of equipment that provides the sights, sounds and even the feel of anairplane in flight. It is called a flight simulator. The simulator was built for filming airplanescenes in movies.

  Milsa Grant of New York holds her dog Lulu Madonna, as she waits to check-in atLaGuardia Airport in New York. (File photo)

  Sandy Alexander lives in Newport Beach, California. He has a disability that requires his two-year-old dog, Doc, to be always at his side. Mr. Alexander took the dog to the flight school toget him ready for plane trips. He says Doc did not like the bumpy part of the flight simulation.

  "When that started he was pretty agitated and looked up at me and wasn't sure what wasgoing on, and I think we are going to be prepared the next time it happens."

  Dog trainer Mary Segall is with Canine Companions for Independence. It provides dogs forpeople with disabilities. Ms. Segall says successful training is based on a simple rule:preparation.

  "Dogs need to be exposed gradually and repetitively to stimulation, to the environment, toloud noises, to sounds and other dogs so that when this experience happens to them on adaily basis, they're able to act in a way that they're used to acting and that they don't getexcitable."

  Stacy Huckeba's nine-month-old dog, Striker, is still in training to be a guide dog.

  "I don't fly a lot so I don't know a lot, but, so I don't know the routine myself. So with a dog Iwould have been lost."

  Dog owners who attended the training at Air Hollywood K9 Flight School say they now feelmuch more at ease about future flights. Their dogs also seem ready for takeoff.

  有關(guān)于狗狗的英語美文篇三

  狗狗就算再溫順 也會(huì)羨慕嫉妒恨

  Any dog owner would testify that dogs are just as prone to jealousy as humans.

  任何狗主人都能作證,狗狗簡直和人一樣容易嫉妒。

  But can one really compare Othello’s agony to Roscoe’s pique?

  但真的有人能把羅斯科(Roscoe)的憤懣與奧賽羅(Othello)的痛苦相提并論嗎?

  The answer, according to Christine Harris, a psychologist at the University of California, San Diego, is that if you are petting another dog, Roscoe is going to show something that Dr. Harris thinks is a form of jealousy, even if not as complex and twisted as the adult human form.

  加州大學(xué)圣迭戈分校(University of California, San Diego)的心理學(xué)家克里斯蒂娜·哈里斯(Christine Harris)表示,答案是,如果你撫摸另一只狗,羅斯科會(huì)有某種表現(xiàn)。哈里斯博士認(rèn)為,這種表現(xiàn)是嫉妒的一種形式,盡管這種形式不像成年人類的嫉妒形式那么復(fù)雜、扭曲。

  Other scientists agree there is something going on, but not all are convinced it is jealousy. And Roscoe and the rest of his tribe were, without exception, unavailable for comment.

  其他一些科學(xué)家也認(rèn)同這種情形下會(huì)出現(xiàn)某種情況,但并不都相信那是嫉妒。而羅斯科和他的同類,無一例外均不便發(fā)表評論。

  Dr. Harris had been studying human jealousy for years when she took this question on, inspired partly by the antics of her parents’ Border collies. When she petted them, “one would take his head and knock the other’s head away,” she said. It certainly looked like jealousy.

  在遇到這個(gè)問題時(shí),哈里斯博士已經(jīng)對人類的嫉妒情緒進(jìn)行了多年研究。當(dāng)時(shí),她部分是受了父母的邊境牧羊犬表現(xiàn)出的怪異行為的啟發(fā)。她說,當(dāng)她撫摸他們時(shí),“其中一只會(huì)用頭把另外一只的頭擠開”。這種情形看上去當(dāng)然像嫉妒了。

  But having studied humans, she was aware of different schools of thought about jealousy. Some scientists argue that jealousy requires complex thinking about self and others, which seems beyond dogs’ abilities. Others think that although our descriptions of jealousy are complex, the emotion itself may not be that complex.

  但因?yàn)橐呀?jīng)對人類進(jìn)行了研究,她了解有關(guān)嫉妒的不同思想流派。一些科學(xué)家認(rèn)為,嫉妒要求有能力對自身和他人進(jìn)行復(fù)雜思考,這似乎超出了狗狗的能力。其他一些人則認(rèn)為,盡管我們對嫉妒的描述頗為復(fù)雜,但這種情感本身或許并不復(fù)雜。

  Dog emotions, as owners perceive them, have been studied before. In one case, Alexandra Horowitz, a cognitive scientist who is an adjunct associate professor at Barnard College and the author of “Inside of a Dog,” found that the so-called guilty look that dogs exhibit seemed to be more related to fear of punishment.

  以前就有人研究過主人眼中狗狗的情感。在其中一項(xiàng)研究中,著有《一只狗的內(nèi)心》(Inside of a Dog)的認(rèn)知科學(xué)家、巴納德學(xué)院(Barnard College)客座副教授亞歷山德拉·霍羅威茨(Alexandra Horowitz)發(fā)現(xiàn),狗狗表現(xiàn)出來的所謂的內(nèi)疚表情似乎與害怕受到懲罰更相關(guān)。

  Dr. Harris ventured into the tricky turf of dog emotion by devising a test based on work done with infants.

  哈里斯博士以針對孩子的研究工作為基礎(chǔ),設(shè)計(jì)了一個(gè)試驗(yàn),大膽進(jìn)入了狗的情感這個(gè)棘手領(lǐng)域。

  When dog owners petted and talked to a realistic stuffed dog that barked and whined, the people’s own dogs came over, pushed the person or the stuffed dog, and sometimes barked. After the experiment, many of the dogs sniffed the rear end of the stuffed dog, suggesting, Dr. Harris said, that the dogs thought it might be real.

  當(dāng)狗主人撫摸一只逼真的、能吠叫并發(fā)出嗚嗚聲的填充玩具狗,并與其說話時(shí),他們自己的狗會(huì)走過來,推自己的主人或那只玩具狗,有時(shí)還會(huì)大叫。試驗(yàn)結(jié)束后,許多狗狗會(huì)嗅那只填充玩具狗的臀部。哈里斯博士稱,這表明,那些狗狗以為玩具狗可能是一只真狗。

  Dr. Harris also recorded what happened as the owners petted and talked to a jack-o’-lantern and read a children’s book aloud, to see if any old distraction would provoke a reaction. The dogs paid little attention to the jack-o’-lantern and very little to the book.

  為了看看隨便什么通常分散注意力的事情會(huì)不會(huì)讓它們有所反應(yīng),哈里斯還記錄了狗主人在撫摸南瓜燈籠并和它說話,以及大聲朗讀兒童讀物時(shí)發(fā)生的情況。那些狗少有注意燈籠的,對那本書,狗狗的關(guān)注度則更小。

  Dr. Harris concluded, in a paper in PLoS One written with Caroline Prouvost, also at the University of California, San Diego, that the dogs showed a “primordial” form of jealousy, not as complex as the human emotion, but similar in that there is a social triangle and the dog is trying to make sure it, not the rival, receives the attention.

  在與同在加州大學(xué)圣迭戈分校任職的卡羅琳·普魯沃(Caroline Prouvost)共同撰寫,并發(fā)表在《公共科學(xué)圖書館·綜合》(PLOS One)的一篇文章中,哈里斯得出結(jié)論稱,那些狗表現(xiàn)出了一種“原始”形式的嫉妒,不像人類的嫉妒那么復(fù)雜。但它也是發(fā)生在一種社會(huì)三角關(guān)系之中,狗狗會(huì)嘗試確定獲得關(guān)注的是自己,而不是對手,因此這種情感與人類的嫉妒類似。

  Other scientists had mixed reactions to the work. Dr. Horowitz said she admired the goal but thought the researchers had not shown that the behaviors observed actually indicated jealousy.

  其他科研人員對這項(xiàng)研究的反響各異?;袅_威茨博士說,她很欣賞該研究的目標(biāo),但她認(rèn)為,研究者沒能證明,被觀察到的行為真的代表嫉妒。

  “What can be shown is that dogs seem to want an owner’s attention when there is attention being given out,” she said. “This study confirms that.”

  “可以被證明的是,當(dāng)主人向外界給予關(guān)注的時(shí)候,狗狗似乎想要得到這種關(guān)注,”她說,“這項(xiàng)研究證實(shí)了這一點(diǎn)。”

  Sybil Hart, at Texas Tech, who has studied jealousy in infants, said she thought the research was “very well done and makes a very compelling argument.”

  德克薩斯理工大學(xué)的西比爾·哈特(Sybil Hart)曾經(jīng)研究過嬰兒的嫉妒心理。哈特說,她認(rèn)為這項(xiàng)研究“做得很出色,觀點(diǎn)令人信服”。

  If one sees jealousy in babies and dogs, she said, “to some degree, it’s innate,” which would be important to know for attempts to manage human jealousy.

  哈特說,如果人們發(fā)現(xiàn)了嬰兒和狗狗的嫉妒之心,“在一定程度上,這是天生的”。了解這一點(diǎn)對于管理成人的嫉妒情緒十分重要。

  “Over all, trying to make it go away has not been very successful,” Dr. Hart said. “We are trying to eliminate jealousy, and scientists are saying maybe we should try to understand it better.”

  “總的來說,消除嫉妒心理的努力并不是非常成功,”哈特博士說。“我們正努力消除嫉妒情緒,而科學(xué)家告訴我們,或許我們應(yīng)該試著更好地理解它。”

  Jealousy, Dr. Harris wrote in the study, is “the third leading cause of nonaccidental homicide across cultures.”

  哈里斯在研究中寫道,嫉妒“在許多文化中都是故意殺人案的第三大誘因”。

  Whatever the dogs’ behavior is called, said Brian Hare, a director of the Duke Canine Cognition Center at Duke University, there are practical implications for their owners.

  杜克大學(xué)杜克犬科動(dòng)物認(rèn)知研究中心(Duke Canine Cognition Center at Duke University)的主任布賴恩·黑爾(Brian Hare)說,無論犬類的這種行為被稱作什么,對他們的主人而言,都會(huì)存在一些實(shí)實(shí)在在的影響。

  “Attention seeking can lead to jealousylike behavior in dogs that includes aggression in some cases,” he said. “So for dogs with suspected aggression problems, it may be important to avoid situations where they feel ignored.”

  “尋求關(guān)注可以引發(fā)狗狗類似于嫉妒的表現(xiàn),比如有時(shí)候出現(xiàn)攻擊性,”他說。“因此,對于可能有攻擊性問題的狗狗來說,避免讓他們感到被忽略也許十分重要。”

  
看了“有關(guān)于狗狗的英語美文”的人還看了:

1.關(guān)于狗的英語美文欣賞

2.有關(guān)于狗狗的英文文章

3.關(guān)于寵物的英語美文閱讀

4.關(guān)于狗狗的英文文章閱讀

5.關(guān)于狗的英語短文閱讀

2191229